enow.com Web Search

Search results

  1. Results from the WOW.Com Content Network
  2. Perception of English /r/ and /l/ by Japanese speakers

    en.wikipedia.org/.../r/_and_/l/_by_Japanese_speakers

    Lively et al. (1994) found that monolingual Japanese speakers in Japan could increase their ability to distinguish between /l/ and /r/ after a 3-week training period, which involved hearing minimal pairs (such as 'rock' and 'lock') produced by five speakers, and being asked to identify which word was which. Feedback was provided during training ...

  3. Minimal pair - Wikipedia

    en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Minimal_pair

    The minimal pair was an essential tool in the discovery process and was found by substitution or commutation tests. [3] As an example for English vowels, the pair "let" + "lit" can be used to demonstrate that the phones [ɛ] (in let) and [ɪ] (in lit) actually represent distinct phonemes /ɛ/ and /ɪ/.

  4. Phonological history of English close back vowels - Wikipedia

    en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Phonological_history_of...

    However, there are few minimal pairs like that, and their use as such has been criticised by scholars such as Geoff Lindsey because the members of such minimal pairs are structurally different. Even so, pairs of words belonging to the same lexical category exist as well such as append / ə ˈ p ɛ n d / vs up-end / ʌ p ˈ ɛ n d / and aneath ...

  5. Contrastive distribution - Wikipedia

    en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Contrastive_distribution

    A contrastive distribution is demonstrated with a minimal pair. Contrastive distribution is distinct from complementary distribution (when two elements cannot be substituted for one another) and free variation (where speech sounds can be substituted without changing the meaning).

  6. There are a few minimal pairs contrasting /ʊə/ and /u:ə/ in conservative Received Pronunciation: dour–doer, sure–shoer and cure-queuer. Furthermore, the hurry – furry merger that occurs in most varieties of North American English results in a merger of /ʌr/ with /ɜr/ , removing almost any trace of the historical foot vowel in this ...

  7. Phoneme - Wikipedia

    en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Phoneme

    The two words 'pressure' / ˈ p r ɛ ʃ ər / and 'pleasure' / ˈ p l ɛ ʒ ər / can serve as a near minimal pair. [4] The reason why this is still acceptable proof of phonemehood is that there is nothing about the additional difference (/r/ vs. /l/) that can be expected to somehow condition a voicing difference for a single underlying ...

  8. Vowel length - Wikipedia

    en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Vowel_length

    In linguistics, vowel length is the perceived length of a vowel sound: the corresponding physical measurement is duration.In some languages vowel length is an important phonemic factor, meaning vowel length can change the meaning of the word, for example in Arabic, Czech, Dravidian languages (such as Tamil), some Finno-Ugric languages (such as Finnish and Estonian), Japanese, Kyrgyz, Samoan ...

  9. Rhotic consonant - Wikipedia

    en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Rhotic_consonant

    The unfavorability of dropping /r/ can be explained with minimal pairs, such as çaldı ('stole') versus çaldır (imperative 'ring'). [citation needed] In some parts of Turkey, like Kastamonu, the syllable-final /r/ is almost never pronounced: gidiya instead of gidiyor ("she/he is going") and gide instead of gider ("she/he goes").